2. What are the best practices that organizations utilize to
train employees in new job skills?
A central feature of most approaches to learning is the concept
of reinforcement. This concept dates from Thorndike’s law of
effect, which, as mentioned earlier, states that behavior that is
positively reinforced tends to be repeated, whereas behavior that
is not reinforced will tend not to be repeated. Hence,
reinforcement can be defined as anything that
causes a certain behavior to be repeated or inhibited.
Reinforcement versus Motivation
It is important to differentiate reinforcement from the concept
of employee motivation. Motivation, as described in the next
chapter, represents a primary psychological process that is largely
cognitive in nature. Thus, motivation is largely internal—it is
experienced by the employee, and we can see only
subsequent manifestations of it in actual behavior. Reinforcement,
on the other hand, is typically observable and most often
externally administered. A supervisor may reinforce what he or she
considers desirable behavior without knowing anything about the
underlying motives that prompted it. For example, a supervisor who
has a habit of saying “That’s interesting” whenever she is
presented with a new idea may be reinforcing innovation on the part
of the subordinates without the supervisor really knowing why this
result is achieved. The distinction between theories of motivation
and reinforcement should be kept in mind when we examine behavior
modification and behavioral self-management later in this
chapter.
Strategies for Behavioral Change
From a managerial standpoint, several strategies for behavioral
change are available to facilitate learning in organizational
settings. At least four different types should be noted: (1)
positive reinforcement; (2) avoidance learning, or negative
reinforcement; (3) extinction; and (4) punishment. Each type plays
a different role in both the manner in which and extent to which
learning occurs. Each will be considered separately here.
Positive Reinforcement. Positive reinforcement
consists of presenting someone with an attractive outcome following
a desired behavior. As noted by Skinner, “A positive reinforcer is
a stimulus which, when added to a situation, strengthens the
probability of an operant response.”11 A simple example
of positive reinforcement is supervisory praise for
subordinates when they perform well in a certain situation. That
is, a supervisor may praise an employee for being on time
consistently (see Exhibit 4.6). This
behavior-praise pattern may encourage the subordinate to be on time
in the future in the hope of receiving additional praise.
Exhibit 4.6 Strategies for Behavioral
Change
In order for a positive reinforcement to be effective in
facilitating the repetition of desired behavior, several conditions
must be met. First, the reinforcer itself (praise) must be valued
by the employee. It would prove ineffective in shaping behavior if
employees were indifferent to it. Second, the reinforcer must be
strongly tied to the desired behavior. Receipt of the reinforcer by
the employee must be directly contingent upon performing the
desired behavior. “Rewards must result from performance, and the
greater the degree of performance by an employee, the greater
should be his reward.”12 It is important to keep in mind
here that “desired behavior” represents behavior defined by the
supervisor, not the employee. Thus, for praise to be a reinforcer,
not only must it be valued by the employee, but it must directly
follow the desired behavior and should be more intense as the
behavior is closer to the ideal the supervisor has in mind. Praise
thrown out at random is unlikely to reinforce the desired behavior.
Third, there must be ample occasion for the reinforcer to be
administered following desired behavior. If the reinforcer is tied
to certain behavior that seldom occurs, then individuals will
seldom be reinforced and will probably not associate this behavior
with a reward. For example, if praise is only provided for truly
exceptional performance, then it is unlikely to have a powerful
impact on the desired behavior. It is important that the
performance-reward contingencies be structured so that they are
easily attainable.
Avoidance Learning. A second method of
reinforcement is avoidance learning, or negative
reinforcement. Avoidance learning refers to seeking to avoid an
unpleasant condition or outcome by following a desired behavior.
Employees learn to avoid unpleasant situations by behaving in
certain ways. If an employee correctly performs a task or is
continually prompt in coming to work (see Exhibit
4.6), the supervisor may refrain from harassing,
reprimanding, or otherwise embarrassing the employee. Presumably,
the employee learns over time that engaging in correct behavior
diminishes admonition from the supervisor. In order to maintain
this condition, the employee continues to behave as desired.
Extinction. The principle of
extinction suggests that undesired behavior will
decline as a result of a lack of positive reinforcement. If the
perpetually tardy employee in the example in Exhibit
4.6 consistently fails to receive supervisory praise and
is not recommended for a pay raise, we would expect this
nonreinforcement to lead to an “extinction” of the tardiness. The
employee may realize, albeit subtly, that being late is not leading
to desired outcomes, and she may try coming to work on time.
Punishment. Finally, a fourth strategy for
behavior change used by managers and supervisors is punishment.
Punishment is the administration of unpleasant or
adverse outcomes as a result of undesired behavior. An example of
the application of punishment is for a supervisor to publicly
reprimand or fine an employee who is habitually tardy (see
Exhibit 4.6). Presumably, the employee would
refrain from being tardy in the future in order to avoid such an
undesirable outcome. The most frequently used punishments (along
with the most frequently used rewards) are shown in Table
4.1.
Table 4.1
The use of punishment is indeed one of the most controversial
issues of behavior change strategies. Although punishment can have
positive work outcomes—especially if it is administered in an
impersonal way and as soon as possible after the
transgression—negative repercussions can also result when employees
either resent the action or feel they are being treated unfairly.
These negative outcomes from punishment are shown in
Exhibit 4.7. Thus, although punishment represents
a potent force in corrective learning, its use must be carefully
considered and implemented. In general, for punishment to be
effective the punishment should “fit the crime” in severity, should
be given in private, and should be explained to the employee.
Exhibit 4.7 Potential Negative Consequences of
Punishment
ethics in practice
Detracting a Workplace Bully
Studies showcase that nearly 50 percent of employees in the U.S.
workforce face bullying at one point in time. All types of
bullying, not just discrimination or harassment, are important to
consider.
Angela Anderson was working for a law school administration
council and experienced bullying firsthand. Often her manager would
yell at her in front of other coworkers, and it was clear to Angela
that she was not well-liked. Unfortunately it was not just Angela
who felt the wrath of this manager, who often handled interactions
with other employees the same way. Many of the employees, including
Angela, attempted to appease their bullying manager, but nothing
would help. One day Angela was threatened by her manager, and
before Angela could reach the HR department, she was fired. This
example is an extreme case, but being able to take recourse against
unwanted and disruptive employee behavior is an important action
for any workplace manager.
Questions:
What steps can you take to ensure that your company can detract
from employees’ bullying behavior?
What actions should an employee take if they are experiencing
unwanted behaviors from another employee or manager?
What other departments should be involved when developing a plan
and policies for how to handle unacceptable workplace behavior?
Sources: Acceptable and Unacceptable Behaviours, University
of Cambridge website, accessed January 15, 2019,
www.hr.admin.cam.ac.uk/polic...ance-managers-
and-staff/guidance-managers/acceptable-and; Hedges, Kristi, How to
Change Your Employee’s Behavior,” Forbes, March, 4, 2015,
www.forbes.com/sites/work-in.../03/04/how-to-
change-your-employees-behavior/#c32ad4b6732a; and Kane, Sally,
Workplace Bullying: True Stories, Statistics and Tips, The
Balance Careers, January 29, 2019, https://www.thebalancecareers.com/bu...tories-2164317.
In summary, positive reinforcement and avoidance learning focus
on bringing about the desired response from the employee.
With positive reinforcement the employee behaves in a certain way
in order to gain desired rewards, whereas with avoidance learning
the employee behaves in order to avoid certain unpleasant outcomes.
In both cases, however, the behavior desired by the supervisor is
enhanced. In contrast, extinction and punishment focus on
supervisory attempts to reduce the incidence of undesired
behavior. That is, extinction and punishment are typically used to
get someone to stop doing something the supervisor doesn’t like. It
does not necessarily follow that the individual will begin acting
in the most desired, or correct, manner.
Often students have difficulty seeing the distinction between
avoidance and extinction or in understanding how either could have
a significant impact on behavior. Two factors are important to keep
in mind. The first we will simply call the “history effect.” Not
being harassed could reinforce an employee’s prompt arrival at work
if in the past the employee had been harassed for being late.
Arriving on time and thereby avoiding the past harassment would
reinforce arriving on time. This same dynamic would hold true for
extinction. If the employee had been praised in the past for
arriving on time, then arrived late and was not praised, this would
serve to weaken the tendency to arrive late. The second factor we
will call the “social effect.” For example, if you see others
harassed when they arrive late and then you are not harassed when
you arrive on time, this could reinforce your arriving at work on
time. Again, this same dynamic would hold true for extinction. If
you had observed others being praised for arriving on time, then
not receiving praise when you arrived late would serve to weaken
the tendency to arrive late.
From a managerial perspective, questions arise about which
strategy of behavioral change is most effective. Advocates of
behavioral change strategies, such as Skinner, answer that positive
reinforcement combined with extinction is the most suitable way to
bring about desired behavior. There are several reasons for this
focus on the positive approach to reinforcement. First, although
punishment can inhibit or eliminate undesired behavior, it often
does not provide information to the individual about how or in
which direction to change. Also, the application of punishment may
cause the individual to become alienated from the work situation,
thereby reducing the chances that useful change can be effected.
Similarly, avoidance learning tends to emphasize the negative; that
is, people are taught to stay clear of certain behaviors, such as
tardiness, for fear of repercussions. In contrast, it is felt that
combining positive reinforcement with the use of extinction has the
fewest undesirable side effects and allows individuals to receive
the rewards they desire. A positive approach to reinforcement is
believed by some to be the most effective tool management has to
bring about favorable changes in organizations.
Schedules of Reinforcement
Having examined four distinct strategies for behavioral change,
we now turn to an examination of the various ways, or
schedules, of administering these techniques. As noted by
Costello and Zalkind, “The speed with which learning takes place
and also how lasting its effects will be is determined by the
timing of reinforcement.”13 Thus, a knowledge of the types of
schedules of reinforcement is essential to managers if they are to
know how to choose rewards that will have maximum impact on
employee performance. Although there are a variety of ways in which
rewards can be administered, most approaches can be categorized
into two groups: continuous and partial (or intermittent)
reinforcement schedules. A continuous
reinforcement schedule rewards desired behavior every time
it occurs. For example, a manager could praise (or pay) employees
every time they perform properly. With the time and resource
constraints most managers work under, this is often difficult,
if not impossible. So, most managerial reward strategies
operate on a partial schedule. A partial
reinforcement schedule rewards desired behavior at
specific intervals, not every time desired behavior is exhibited.
Compared to continuous schedules, partial reinforcement schedules
lead to slower learning but stronger retention. Thus, learning is
generally more permanent. Four kinds of partial reinforcement
schedules can be identified: (1) fixed interval, (2) fixed ratio,
(3) variable interval, and (4) variable ratio (see Table
4.2).
Table 4.2
Fixed-Interval Schedule. A fixed-interval
reinforcement schedule rewards individuals at specified intervals
for their performance, as with a biweekly paycheck. If employees
perform even minimally, they are paid. This technique generally
does not result in high or sustained levels of performance because
employees know that marginal performance usually leads to the same
level of reward as high performance. Thus, there is little
incentive for high effort and performance. Also, when rewards are
withheld or suspended, extinction of desired behavior occurs
quickly. Many of the recent job redesign efforts in organizations
were prompted by recognition of the need for alternate strategies
of motivation rather than paying people on fixed-interval
schedules.
Fixed-Ratio Schedule. The second fixed schedule
is the fixed-ratio schedule. Here the reward is administered only
upon the completion of a given number of desired responses. In
other words, rewards are tied to performance in a ratio of rewards
to results. A common example of the fixed-ratio schedule is a
piece-rate pay system, whereby employees are paid for each
unit of output they produce. Under this system, performance rapidly
reaches high levels. In fact, according to Hamner, “The response
level here is significantly higher than that obtained under any of
the interval (time-based) schedules.”14 On the negative
side, however, performance declines sharply when the rewards are
withheld, as with fixed-interval schedules.
Variable-Interval Schedule. Using variable
reinforcement schedules, both variable-interval and variable-ratio
reinforcements are administered at random times that cannot be
predicted by the employee. The employee is generally not aware of
when the next evaluation and reward period will be. Under a
variable-interval schedule, rewards are administered at intervals
of time that are based on an average. For example, an employee may
know that on the average her performance is evaluated and
rewarded about once a month, but she does not know when this event
will occur. She does know, however, that it will occur sometime
during the interval of a month. Under this schedule, effort and
performance will generally be high and fairly stable over time
because employees never know when the evaluation will take
place.
Variable-Ratio Schedule. Finally, a
variable-ratio schedule is one in which rewards are administered
only after an employee has performed the desired behavior a number
of times, with the number changing from the administration of one
reward to the next but averaging over time to a certain
ratio of number of performances to rewards. For example, a
manager may determine that a salesperson will receive a bonus for
every 15th new account sold. However, instead of administering the
bonus every 15th sale (as in a fixed-interval schedule), the
manager may vary the number of sales that is necessary for the
bonus, from perhaps 10 sales for the first bonus to 20 for the
second. On the average, however, the 15:1 ratio prevails. If the
employee understands the parameters, then the “safe” level of
sales, or the level of sales most likely to result in a bonus, is
in excess of 15. Consequently, the variable-ratio schedule
typically leads to high and stable performance. Moreover,
extinction of desired behavior is slow.
Which of these four schedules of reinforcement is superior? In a
review of several studies comparing the various techniques, Hamner
concludes:
The necessity for arranging appropriate reinforcement
contingencies is dramatically illustrated by several studies in
which rewards were shifted from a response-contingent (ratio) to a
time-contingent (interval) basis. During the period in which
rewards were made conditional upon occurrence of the desired
behavior, the appropriate response patterns were exhibited at a
consistently high level. When the same rewards were given based on
time and independent of the worker’s behavior, there was a marked
drop in the desired behavior. The reinstatements of the
performance-contingent reward schedule promptly restored the high
level of responsiveness.
In other words, the performance-contingent (or ratio) reward
schedules generally lead to better performance than the
time-contingent (or interval) schedules, regardless of whether such
schedules are fixed or variable. We will return to this point in a
subsequent chapter on performance appraisal and reward systems.
Two additional approaches to learning are found in the work of
David Kolb and Mel Silberman. Kolb's experiential learning style
theory is typically represented by a four-stage learning cycle in
which the learner 'touches all the bases’. The Four stages are
achieved when a person progresses through a cycle of four stages:
of (1) having a concrete experience followed by (2) observation of
and reflection on that experience which leads to (3) the formation
of abstract concepts (analysis) and generalizations (conclusions)
which are then (4) used to test hypothesis in future situations,
resulting in new experiences. Silberman in his book Active
Training, identified eight qualities of an effective and
active learning experience. The eight qualities are: a moderate
level of content; a balance between affective, behavioral, and
cognitive learning, a variety of learning approaches, opportunities
for group participation, encouraging participants to share their
expertise, recycling concepts and skills learned earlier,
advocating real-life problem solving, and allowing time for
re-entry.15
managerial leadership
Shaping a Salesperson’s Behavior
Sharon Johnson worked for a publishing company based in
Nashville, Tennessee, that sold a line of children’s books directly
to the public through a door-to-door sales force. Sharon had been a
very successful salesperson and was promoted first to district and
then to regional sales manager after just four years with the
company. Sales bonuses were fixed, and a fixed-dollar bonus was
tied to every $1,000 in sales over a specific minimum quota.
However, there was a wide variety of rewards, from praise to gift
certificates, that were left to Sharon’s discretion.
Sharon knew from her organizational behavior class that giving
out praise to those who liked it and gifts to those who preferred
them was an important means of reinforcing desired behavior, and
she had been quite successful in implementing this principle. She
also knew that if you reinforced a behavior that was “on the right
track” to the ideal behavior you wanted out of a salesperson,
eventually you could shape their behavior, almost without their
realizing it.
Sharon had one particular salesperson, Lyle, that she thought
had great potential, yet his weekly sales were somewhat
inconsistent and often lower than she thought possible. When Lyle
was questioned about his performance, he indicated that sometimes
he felt that the families he approached could not afford the books
he was selling and so he did not think it was right to push the
sale too hard. Although Sharon argued that it was not Lyle’s place
to decide for others what they could or could not afford, Lyle
still felt uncomfortable about utilizing his normal sales approach
with these families.
Sharon believed that through subtle reinforcement of certain
behaviors she could shape Lyle’s behavior and that over time he
would increasingly use his typical sales approach with the families
he thought could not afford the books. For example, she knew that
in the cases of families Lyle thought could not afford the books,
he spent only 3.5 minutes in the house compared to 12.7 minutes in
homes of families he judged able to afford the books. Sharon
believed that if she praised Lyle when the average time he spent in
each family’s home was quite similar that Lyle would increase the
time he spent in the homes of families he judged unable to afford
the books. She believed that the longer he spent in these homes,
the more likely Lyle was to utilize his typical sales approach.
This was just one of several ways Sharon thought she could shape
Lyle’s behavior without trying to change his mind about pushing
books onto people he thought could not afford them.
Sharon saw no ethical issues in this case until she told a
friend about it and the friend questioned whether it was ethical to
utilize learning and reinforcement techniques to change people’s
behavior “against their will” even if they did not realize that
this was happening.
Source: This ethical challenge is based on a true but disguised
case observed by author J. Stewart Black.
concept check
What is reinforcement, and how can it be applied to
motivation?
What are the four strategies to use for behavioral change?
What is the significance of schedules in changing behavior?
11. B. F. Skinner, Science and Human Behavior (New
York: Macmillan, 1953), p. 73.
12. W. C. Hamner, “Reinforcement Theory,” in H. L. Tosi and W.
C. Hamner, eds., Organizational Behavior and Management: A
Contingency Approach (Chicago: St. Clair, 1977), p. 98.
13. T. W. Costello and S. S. Zalkind, Psychology in
Administration: A Research Orientation (Englewood Cliffs, N.
J.: Prentice-Hall, 1963), p. 193.
14. Hamner, op. cit., p. 105.
15. David Kolb, Experiential Learning, 2nd Edition,
(Pearson FT Press: New York, 2015) and Mel Silberman, Elaine Beich
and Carol Auerbach, Active Training, (Wiley: New York,
2016).
Table 4.1 (Attribution: Copyright Rice University, OpenStax,
under CC BY-NC-SA 4.0 license)
Table 4.2 (Attribution: Copyright Rice University, OpenStax,
under CC BY-NC-SA 4.0 license)
Exhibit 4.6 Strategies for Behavioral Change (Attribution:
Copyright Rice University, OpenStax, under CC BY-NC-SA 4.0
license)
Exhibit 4.7 Potential Negative Consequences of Punishment
(Attribution: Copyright Rice University, OpenStax, under CC
BY-NC-SA 4.0 license)