Google is great at many things—attracting top talent,
maintaining employee satisfaction, and encouraging creativity, to
name a few.
According to the Association of Training and Development (ATD),
companies that offer comprehensive training programs have 218
percent higher income per employee than companies without
formalized training. Not only that, but companies that have
required programs for their employees see a much higher profit
margin than those that don’t. Investing in people and promoting a
self-learning environment is the right plan for companies that are
looking to keep employees’ behavior in check, train or new
skills, and increase employee development.
Spending millions of dollars is not necessary to create a
culture that promotes learning.
Google follows the simple principles that gives their employees
purpose and a career path. They provide information that is
relevant and important to their employees. They know that in order
to get this information to stick, it must be pertinent and
presented at the right time, and in the right format. They also
archive important information, which empowers employees to access
this information at any and all times. Instead of providing
gateways that impede learning, they open the doors.
Secondly, they share “dumb questions.” This may seem like a
silly tactic, but encouraging employees to share their questions
and opinions allows for sharing of information and learning on all
levels. Google also employs the values of celebrated failure, which
allows for the teams to learn from their mistakes and their
failures. Then they can move on to the next project with newly
found valuable information to get better each time.
Lastly, formalized plans for continued learning are employed for
“informal and continuous learning” to occur. Examples of these
events can be allowing employees to pursue their own interests,
utilizing coaching and support tools, and then training being
requested at various times. With these tactics, the cultivation of
learning can be expressed throughout the company. Google is at the
forefront of this pursuit, but other companies can learn from their
methods to get ahead and get their employees on track as well.
Questions:
What considerations should Google take into account when
creating formalized training for their employees?
Name three reasons why training and continued learning can be
important for a company’s success.
Why is encouraging and celebrating failure an important thing
for a company to promote?
A major responsibility of managers is to evaluate and reward
their subordinates. If managers are to maximize the impact of
available (and often limited) rewards, a thorough knowledge of
reinforcement techniques is essential. We shall devote this chapter
to developing a detailed understanding of learning processes in
organizations. We begin by looking at basic models of learning.
Sources: Ault, Nicole, “Don’t Trust Anyone Over 21,” The
Wall Street Journal, August 22, 2018,
www.wsj.com/articles/dont-trust-anyone-
over-21-1534977740?mod=searchresults&page=1&pos=1; and
Gutierrez, Karla, “Mind-blowing Statistics that Prove the Value of
Employee Training and Development, Shift, August 22, 2017,
https://www.shiftelearning.com/blog/...nd-development.
1. How do organizations offer appropriate rewards in a timely
fashion?
Learning may be defined, for our purposes, as a relatively
permanent change in behavior that occurs as a result of experience.
That is, a person is said to have learned something when she
consistently exhibits a new behavior over time. Several
aspects of this definition are noteworthy.1 First, learning
involves a change in an attitude or behavior. This change does
not necessarily have to be an improvement, however, and can include
such things as learning bad habits or forming prejudices. In order
for learning to occur, the change that takes place must be
relatively permanent. So changes in behavior that result from
fatigue or temporary adaptation to a unique situation would not be
considered examples of learning. Next, learning typically involves
some form of practice or experience. For example, the change that
results from physical maturation, as when a baby develops the
physical strength to walk, is in itself not considered learning.
Third, this practice or experience must be reinforced over time for
learning to take place. Where reinforcement does not follow
practice or experience, the behavior will eventually diminish and
disappear (“extinction”). Finally, learning is an inferred process;
we cannot observe learning directly. Instead, we must infer the
existence of learning from observing changes in overt
behavior.
We can best understand the learning process by looking at four
stages in the development of research on learning (see
Exhibit 4.2). Scientific interest in learning
dates from the early experiments of Pavlov and others around the
turn of the century. The focus of this research was on
stimulus-response relationships and the environmental determinants
of observable behaviors. This was followed by the discovery of the
law of effect, experiments in operant conditioning, and, finally,
the formulation of social learning theory.
Exhibit 4.2 The Development of Modern Behavioral
Learning Theory
Classical Conditioning
Classical conditioning is the process whereby a
stimulus-response (S-R) bond is developed between a conditioned
stimulus and a conditioned response through the
repeated linking of a conditioned stimulus with an
unconditioned stimulus. This process is shown in Exhibit
4.3. The classic example of Pavlov’s experiments
illustrates the process. Pavlov was initially interested in the
digestive processes of dogs but noticed that the dogs started to
salivate at the first signal of approaching food. On the basis of
this discovery, he shifted his attention to the question of whether
animals could be trained to draw a causal relationship between
previously unconnected factors. Specifically, using the dogs as
subjects, he examined the extent to which the dogs could learn to
associate the ringing of a bell with the act of salivation. The
experiment began with unlearned, or unconditioned,
stimulus-response relationships. When a dog was presented with meat
(unconditioned stimulus), the dog salivated (unconditioned
response). No learning was necessary here, as this relationship
represented a natural physiological process.
Exhibit 4.3 Classical versus Operant
Conditioning
Next, Pavlov paired the unconditioned stimulus (meat) with a
neutral one (the ringing of a bell). Normally, the ringing of the
bell by itself would not be expected to elicit salivation. However,
over time, a learned linkage developed for the dog between the bell
and meat, ultimately resulting in an S-R bond between the
conditioned stimulus (the bell) and the response (salivation)
without the presence of the unconditioned stimulus (the meat).
Evidence emerged that learning had occurred and that this learning
resulted from conditioning the dogs to associate two normally
unrelated objects, the bell and the meat.
Although Pavlov’s experiments are widely cited as evidence of
the existence of classical conditioning, it is necessary from the
perspective of organizational behavior to ask how this process
relates to people at work. Ivancevich, Szilagyi, and Wallace
provide one such work-related example of classical
conditioning:
An illustration of classical conditioning in a work setting
would be an airplane pilot learning how to use a newly installed
warning system. In this case the behavior to be learned is to
respond to a warning light that indicates that the plane has
dropped below a critical altitude on an assigned glide path. The
proper response is to increase the plane’s altitude. The pilot
already knows how to appropriately respond to the trainer’s warning
to increase altitude (in this case we would say the trainer’s
warning is an unconditioned stimulus and the corrective action of
increasing altitude is an unconditioned response).
The training session consists of the trainer warning the pilot
to increase altitude every time the warning light goes on. Through
repeated pairings of the warning light with the trainer’s warning,
the pilot eventually learns to adjust the plane’s altitude in
response to the warning light even though the trainer is not
present. Again, the unit of learning is a new S-R connection, or
habit.2
Although classical conditioning clearly has applications to work
situations, particularly in the area of training and development,
it has been criticized as explaining only a limited part of total
human learning. Psychologist B. F. Skinner argues that classical
conditioning focuses on respondent, or reflexive, behaviors; that
is, it concentrates on explaining largely involuntary
responses that result from stimuli.3 More complex
learning cannot be explained solely by classical conditioning.
As an alternative explanation, Skinner and others have proposed the
operant conditioning model of learning.
Operant Conditioning
The major focus of operant conditioning is on
the effects of reinforcements, or rewards, on desired behaviors.
One of the first psychologists to examine such processes was J. B.
Watson, a contemporary of Pavlov, who argued that behavior is
largely influenced by the rewards one receives as a result of
actions.4 This notion is best summarized in
Thorndike’s law of effect. This law states that of
several responses made to the same situation, those that are
accompanied or closely followed by satisfaction (reinforcement)
will be more likely to occur; those that are accompanied or closely
followed by discomfort (punishment) will be less likely to
occur.5
In other words, it posits that behavior that leads to positive
or pleasurable outcomes tends to be repeated, whereas behavior that
leads to negative outcomes or punishment tends to be avoided. In
this manner, individuals learn appropriate, acceptable responses to
their environment. If we repeatedly dock the pay of an employee who
is habitually tardy, we would expect that employee to learn to
arrive early enough to receive a full day’s pay.
A basic operant model of learning is presented in
Exhibit 4.2. There are three important concepts of
this model:
Drive. A drive is an internal
state of disequilibrium; it is a felt need. It is generally
believed that drive increases with the strength of deprivation. A
drive, or desire, to learn must be present for learning to take
place. For example, not currently being able to afford the house
you want is likely to lead to a drive for more money to buy your
desired house. Living in a run-down shack is likely to increase
this drive compared to living in a nice apartment.
Habit. A habit is the
experienced bond or connection between stimulus and response. For
example, if a person learns over time that eating satisfies hunger,
a strong stimulus-response (hunger-eating) bond will develop.
Habits thus determine the behaviors, or courses of action, we
choose.
Reinforcement or reward. This represents the
feedback individuals receive as a result of action. For example, if
as a salesperson you are given a bonus for greater sales and plan
to use the money to buy the house you have always wanted, this will
reinforce the behaviors that you believed led to greater sales,
such as smiling at customers, repeating their name during the
presentation, and so on.
A stimulus activates an individual’s motivation through its
impact on drive and habit. The stronger the drive and habit (S-R
bond), the stronger the motivation to behave in a certain way. As a
result of this behavior, two things happen. First, the individual
receives feedback that reduces the original drive. Second, the
individual strengthens his or her belief in the veracity of the S-R
bond to the extent that it proved successful. That is,
if one’s response to the stimulus satisfied one’s drive or
need, the individual would come to believe more strongly in the
appropriateness of the particular S-R connection and would respond
in the same way under similar circumstances.
An example will clarify this point. Several recent attempts to
train chronically unemployed workers have used a daily pay system
instead of weekly or monthly systems. The primary reason for this
is that the workers, who do not have a history of working, can more
quickly see the relationship between coming to work and receiving
pay. An S-R bond develops more quickly because of the frequency of
the reinforcement, or reward.
Operant versus Classical Conditioning
Operant conditioning can be distinguished from classical
conditioning in at least two ways.6 First, the
two approaches differ in what is believed to cause changes in
behavior. In classical conditioning, changes in behavior are
thought to arise through changes in stimuli—that is, a transfer
from an unconditioned stimulus to a conditioned stimulus. In
operant conditioning, on the other hand, changes in behavior are
thought to result from the consequences of previous
behavior. When behavior has not been rewarded or has been punished,
we would not expect it to be repeated.
Second, the two approaches differ in the role and frequency of
rewards. In classical conditioning, the unconditioned stimulus,
acting as a sort of reward, is administered during every trial. In
contrast, in operant conditioning the reward results only when
individuals choose the correct response. That is, in operant
conditioning, individuals must correctly operate on their
environment before a reward is received. The response is
instrumental in obtaining the desired reward.
Social Learning Theory
The last model of learning we should examine is noted
psychologist Albert Bandura’s social learning theory.
Social learning theory is defined as the process
of molding behavior through the reciprocal interaction of a
person's cognitions, behavior, and environment.7 This is
done through a process that Bandura
calls reciprocal determinism. This
concept implies that people control their own environment (for
example, by quitting one’s job) as much as the environment controls
people (for example, being laid off). Thus, learning is seen as a
more active, interactive process in which the learner has at least
some control.
Social learning theory shares many of the same roots as operant
conditioning. Like Skinner, Bandura argues that behavior is at
least in part controlled by environmental cues and consequences,
and Bandura uses observable behavior (as opposed to attitudes,
feelings, etc.) as the primary unit of analysis. However, unlike
operant conditioning, social learning theory posits that cognitive
or mental processes affect our response to the environmental
cues.
Social learning theory has four central elements: attention,
retention, reproduction, and incentives. Before someone can learn
something, they must notice or pay attention to the thing that is
to be learned. For example, you probably would not learn much as a
student in any class unless you paid attention to information
conveyed by the text or instructor. Retention is the process by
which what you have noticed is encoded into your memory.
Reproduction involves the translation of what was recorded in your
mind into overt actions or behaviors. Obviously, the higher the
level of attention and the greater the retention, the better the
reproduction of what was learned. Finally, incentives can influence
all three processes. For example, if you are rewarded (say,
praised) for paying attention, you will pay more attention. If you
are rewarded for remembering what you studied (say, good grades),
you will retain more. If you are rewarded for reproducing what
you learned (say, a promotion for effectively motivating your
subordinates), you will produce that behavior more.
Central to this theory is the concept of vicarious learning.
Vicarious learning is learning that takes place
through the imitation of other role models. That is, we observe and
analyze what another person does and the resulting consequences. As
a result, we learn without having to experience the phenomenon
firsthand. Thus, if we see a fellow employee being disciplined or
fired for being disruptive in the workplace, we might learn not to
be disruptive ourselves. If we see that gifts are usually given
with the right hand in the Middle East, we might give gifts in that
manner ourselves.
A model of social learning processes is shown in Exhibit
4.4. As can be seen, three factors—the person, the
environment, and the behavior—interact through such processes as
vicarious learning, symbolic representations, and self-control to
cause actual learned behaviors.
Exhibit 4.4 A Basic Model of Social
Learning
Major Influences on Learning. On the basis of
this work, it is possible by way of summary to identify several
general factors that can enhance our learning processes. An
individual’s desire to learn, background knowledge of a subject,
and the length of the learning period are some of the components of
a learning environment. Filley, House, and Kerr identify five
major influences on learning effectiveness.8
Drawn largely from behavioral science and psychology literature,
substantial research indicates that learning effectiveness is
increased considerably when individuals have high motivation to
learn. We sometimes encounter students who work day and night
to complete a term paper that is of interest to them,
whereas writing an uninteresting term paper may be postponed
until the last possible minute. Maximum transfer of knowledge is
achieved when a student or employee is motivated to learn by a high
need to know.
Considerable evidence also demonstrates that we can facilitate
learning by providing individuals with feedback on their
performance. A knowledge of results serves a gyroscopic
function, showing individuals where they are correct or incorrect
and furnishing them with the perspective to improve. Feedback also
serves as an important positive reinforcer that can enhance an
individual’s willingness or desire to learn. Students who are told
by their professor how they performed on an exam and what they
could do to improve next time are likely to study harder.
In many cases, prior learning can increase the ability
to learn new materials or tasks by providing needed background or
foundation materials. In math, multiplication is easier to learn if
addition has been mastered. These beneficial effects of prior
learning on present learning tend to be greatest when the prior
tasks and the present tasks exhibit similar stimulus-response
connections. For instance, most of the astronauts selected for the
space program have had years of previous experience flying
airplanes. It is assumed that their prior experience and developed
skill will facilitate learning to fly the highly technical, though
somewhat similar, vehicles.
Another influence on learning concerns whether the materials to
be learned are presented in their entirety or in
parts—whole versus part learning.9 Available
evidence suggests that when a task consists of several
distinct and unrelated duties, part learning is more
effective. Each task should be learned separately. However, when a
task consists of several integrated and related parts (such as
learning the components of a small machine), whole learning is more
appropriate, because it ensures that major relationship among
parts, as well as proper sequencing of parts, is not overlooked or
underemphasized.
Exhibit 4.5 Stop sign in Quebec
The final major influence on learning highlights the advantages
and disadvantages of concentrated as opposed to distributed
training sessions. Research suggests that distribution of
practice—short learning periods at set intervals—is more
effective for learning motor skills than for learning verbal or
cognitive skills. Distributed practice also seems to facilitate
learning of very difficult, voluminous, or tedious material. It
should be noted, however, that concentrated practice appears to
work well where insight is required for task completion.
Apparently, concentrated effort over short durations provides a
move synergistic approach to problem-solving.
Although there is general agreement that these influences are
important (and are under the control of management in many cases),
they cannot substitute for the lack of an adequate reinforcement
system. In fact, reinforcement is widely recognized as the key to
effective learning. If managers are concerned with eliciting
desired behaviors from their subordinates, a knowledge of
reinforcement techniques is essential.
expanding around the globe
Learning to Be Effective Overseas
General Motors has learned by experience that it pays not to
have managers learn only by experience how to function effectively
while working in foreign countries. Managing expatriate assignments
in difficult locations was brought to life by the experiences of
Richard Pennington, General Motors’ head of global mobility for the
EMEA (Europe, Middle East, and Africa) region. He knows from
experience some of the things that tend to go well, as well as some
of those that don’t, and has learned lessons from moving employees
to places like Uzbekistan. This became important when the company
took on a new engine manufacturing operation in the capital,
Tashkent, as well as an existing manufacturing plant in Andijan.
The objectives were the same as for most global mobility projects:
to get the right people to the right place at the right time for
the right cost. The general approach was Action—Plan—Do—Check.
Pennington urged potential relocation candidates not to be
overreliant on the Internet and, if possible, to go and see for
themselves. “Nothing beats going to a location—particularly a harsh
location—yourself,” he says. Pennington also emphasizes the
importance of selecting suppliers on the ground carefully, even if
you already have a network of existing suppliers. Strong
relationships in the host location are of paramount importance. In
difficult locations, it is particularly important that the local
HR, finance, and legal staff work with you proactively, as making
payments at the right time can be critical. Equally, cultural
training and language providers are essential.
These training programs involve a wide variety of teaching
methods. Factual information may be conveyed through lectures or
printed material. More subtle information is learned through role
plays, case studies, and simulations.
The research on cross-cultural training suggests that the more
involved participants are in the training, the more they learn, and
that the more they practice or simulate new behaviors that they
need to master in the foreign environment, the more effective they
will be in actual situations.
The results for GM have been impressive. Most companies that do
not provide cross-cultural training for their employees sent on
international assignments experience failure rates of about 25
percent, and each failure or early return costs the company on
average $150,000. GM has a failure rate of less than 1 percent.
Also, in GM’s case, the training has been extended to the manager’s
family and has helped reluctant spouses and children more readily
accept, if not embrace, the foreign assignment.
Sources: F. Furnie, “International assignments: Managing change
and complexity,” Relocate Global, September 23, 2015,
www.relocatemagazine.com/art...l-assignments-
managing-change-and-complexity; J. Lublin. “Companies Use
Cross-Cultural Training to Help Their Employees Adjust Abroad.”
Wall Street Journal, August 4, 2004 p. B1.
concept check
How can learning theory be used to change behaviors?
Define classical conditioning, and differentiate it from operant
conditioning.
What is social learning theory?
1. Rose E. Spielman, Kathryn Dumper, William Jenkins, Arlene
Lacombe, Marilyn Lovett, and Marion Perlmutter, Psychology
(Houston: OpenStax, 2015).
2. J. M. Ivancevich, A. D. Szilagyi, and M. Wallace,
Organizational Behavior and Performance (Glenview, Ill.:
Scott, Foresman, 1977), p. 80.
3. B. F. Skinner, “Operant Behavior,” American
Psychologist, 1963, 18, pp. 503–515.
4. J. B. Watson, Behavior: An Introduction to Comparative
Psychology (New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, 1914).
5. E. L. Thorndike, Animal Intelligence (New York:
Macmillan, 1911), p. 244.
6. F. Luthans, et. al., Organizational Behavior 13th
Edition (Charlotte: Information Age Publishing, 2016).
7. A. Bandura, Social Learning Theory (Englewood
Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, 1977).
8. A. Filley, R. J. House, and S. Kerr, Managerial Process
and Organizational Behavior (Glenview, III.: Scott, Foresman,
1975).
9. E. J. McCormick and D. Illgen, Industrial Psychology
8th edition (Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, 1984). 10. B.
M. Bass and J. Vaughn, Training in Industry: The Management of
Learning (Belmont, Ca.: Wadsworth, 1966); G. Wexley and G. P.
Latham, Developing and Training Human Resources in
Organizations, Third Edition (Pearson: 2002); and G. P.
Latham, “Human Resource Training and Development,” in M. Rosenzweig
and L. W. Porter, eds., Annual Review of Psychology (Palo
Alto: Annual Reviews, 1988), pp. 545–581.
Exhibit 4.2 The Development of Modern Behavioral Learning Theory
(Attribution: Copyright Rice University, OpenStax, under CC
BY-NC-SA 4.0 license)
Exhibit 4.3 Classical versus Operant Conditioning (Attribution:
Copyright Rice University, OpenStax, under CC BY-NC-SA 4.0
license)
Exhibit 4.4 A Basic Model of Social Learning Source:
Adapted from “A Social Learning Approach to Behavioral Management:
Radical Behaviorists ‘Mellowing Out,’ ” by Robert Kreitner et al.
Organizational Dynamics. (Attribution: Copyright Rice
University, OpenStax, under CC BY- NC-SA 4.0 license)
Exhibit 4.5 Stop sign in Quebec Would your prior learning lead
you to come to a full stop while driving in Quebec, just north of
New York State? (Credit: Joe Schlabotnik/ flickr/ Attribution 2.0
Generic (CC BY 2.0))